Showing posts with label neuroimaging. Show all posts
Showing posts with label neuroimaging. Show all posts

Thursday, April 18, 2013

On Dead Salmon, Drugs, and “Lighting Up” the Brain


Are fMRIs truly useful in addiction medicine?

What would it take to make neuroimaging a truly valuable tool for addiction medicine? Pictures of brain regions “lighting up” have always been exciting, as the early phase of neuroimaging predictably inspired rapture. Phase 2 arrived when a group of U.S. postdocs created the infamous dead salmon fMRI scan, showing that an exciting and colorful picture of false positives was entirely possible. As Neuroskeptic put it to the Globe and Mail, “Scientific journals prefer to publish results that are positive and ‘sexy,’ just like other media.”

That is nice to hear, since it takes the full blast of the heat lamp off journalists and directs it at those scientists with a habit of overamping MRI studies, even when the sample in the studies is exceedingly small. Plenty of blame to go around. Moreover, both scientists and journalists must contend with the fact that the bulk of the scientific world’s research resides behind steep pay walls—steep enough that even prestigious universities have been wailing lately about the cost of just getting one’s hands on the research reports, let along doing the research. “Media literacy in science journalism is really stunted by the fact that we don’t have access to primary sources,” said a spokesperson for the Electronic Frontier Foundation.

So much blame going around, in fact, that enthusiasm for President Obama’s recently announced brain initiative seems particularly muted among one group universally expected to rally around the project—neuroscientists themselves. Having helped to create the hype, some brain scientists are now suggesting that the only appropriate attitude is healthy skepticism about where the money will be used, and whose pockets will be picked to come up with the $100 million in kickoff funds.  Rather than jumping in unison when Obama said the program would allow us to “better understand how we think and how we learn and how we remember,” skeptical neuroscientists note that “Manhattan Project”-style programs are out of fashioned in today’s distributed, system-wide landscape of experiment. “Without specific goals, hypotheses, or endpoints,” said an Emory University neuroscientist in the Globe and Mail article, “the research effort becomes a fishing expedition.”

Myself, I like to fish. But not if the pond’s too small. In a recent post at National Geographic’s blog, “Not Exactly Rocket Science,” Ed Yong quoted a neuroscientist at the University of Bristol: “If you have lots of people running studies that are too small to get a clear answer, that’s more wasteful in the long-term.”

Exactly so, and one might think that a large, coordinated, possibly international initiative at studying the architecture and function of the human brain might serve as a powerful antidote to a micro-universe of tiny studies and insignificant findings.

But forget the big and little pictures for a moment. Let’s focus on what’s in it for addiction studies. What would have to happen—how would fMRIs, PETs and EEGs have to be used in order to advance our understanding of drug and alcohol abuse?

In a recent editorial —“What neuroimaging has and has not yet added to our understanding of addiction”—Martina Reske of the Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine in Julich, Germany, argues that we must take “three critical steps to implement neuroimaging as a new basis for diagnostics and treatment of substance use disorders: first, we need to merge diverse imaging findings into one comprehensive brain imaging perspective of addiction. Next, we need to identify prediction algorithms for individual substance users.” And finally, Reske writes in Addiction, “The ultimate goal has to be the development of treatment regimens based on neuroimaging results.” The interested lay public may be forgiven for assuming that all three of these conditions were already being met.

Specifically, Reske argues for “multi-modal approaches to overcome technological shortcomings. Simultaneous EEG-fMRI, for instance, combines high temporal and spatial resolution of exactly the same mental process, and hybrid MR-PET imaging allows for functional/structural and molecular characterizations.” What might stand in the way of such solutions, you ask? Reske answers that it is likely to be “the existing researchers’ hesitation, unwillingness or inability to consolidate findings from different imaging modalities.” In this case, she suggests, it is the scientists themselves, perhaps overly protective of individual turfs and research fiefdoms, who are hemming and hawing about large-scale collaborative efforts.

To reach a level of clinical relevance for addiction, neuroimaging must be used to delineate and identify “occasional versus habitual versus compulsive use or intoxication versus abstinence versus relapse.” These are not things that existing neuroimagery can do for us, but Reske believes one promising avenue will be the identification of subjects with an abnormally high risk for relapse, something neither patients nor therapists are very good at predicting. (This immediately brings neuroimaging up against a ripe field of ethical questions having to do with the identification and disclosure of high-risk subjects.)

What other payoffs might there be? Reske can think of a few: “First, linkage of neuroimaging and pharmacological studies will prove useful for predicting response to medication. Secondly, knowledge of the biological differences between responders and non-responders to available treatments might facilitate identification of the best-suited therapy for that particular individual. Thirdly, understanding which brain regions show alterations in functioning should spur the development of specific medications, cognitive-behavioral or neuroimaging-based trainings that target optimal activation levels in these regions.”

Neuroimaging is not yet specific or sensitive enough, and its practitioners not yet practiced enough, to accomplish these tasks except in a tantalizingly patchwork fashion. Neuroimaging-based predictions of addiction liability and damage and relapse make up an infant science, ripe for both growth and abuse. Obviously, it will take the gold standard of longitudinal studies involving enormous samples of participants, who would ideally be followed and scanned for decades. But such studies are, as Reske reminds us, “methodologically challenging, expensive and not promising in terms of short-term publication of results.” It sounds like the kind of Big Project that might fit under the umbrella of, say, a major, well-funded, multi-year brain research initiative endorsed by the President of the United States….

Photo Credit: https://docs.uabgrid.uab.edu/

Wednesday, October 19, 2011

An Interview with Neuroscientist Jon Simons


Brain scans, iPhone love, and state-dependent memory.

(Third post in the “Five-Question Interview” series.)

Brain scans have put cognitive neuroscience on the map. They have become a key part of addiction studies as well. In fact, brain scans have put neuroscience on the front page, due to the controversies they have engendered. Cognitive neuroscientist Jon Simons, a lecturer in the Department of Experimental Psychology at the University of Cambridge, UK, and principal investigator at the University’s Memory Laboratory, is attempting to expand our understanding of the specific regions of the brain involved in human memory. His research involves functional neuroimaging of healthy volunteers and examining the effects of neurological and psychiatric disorders, and normal aging, on memory abilities.

Dr. Simons obtained his PhD at the MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit in Cambridge, and from there moved to a post-doctoral position at Harvard University. He returned to the UK and took up a research fellowship at University College London before returning to Cambridge. He was recently senior author on a thought-provoking paper published in the Journal of Neuroscience about a brain structure variation that might explain why some people in the general population are better than others at distinguishing real events from those they imagined or were told about. We asked Dr. Simons to help straighten out some of the confusion over scanning data, and to be the third guest in our “Five-Question Interview” series.

1. PET and fMRI scans have stirred up a good deal of debate and heated argument lately. While brain scans have been used to extend our understanding of crucial functions like memory and reward, they’ve also been used to “prove” that we’re addicted to our iPhones. Some scientists put almost no faith in them at all. What’s going on?

Neuroimaging methods like fMRI have certainly become quite common in the media over recent years. Unfortunately, not all the media coverage does the kind of job we might wish in explaining the methods and findings and, importantly, the limitations and caveats that need to be considered when interpreting the data. You mention the recent New York Times op-ed in which it was claimed that people “literally love their iPhones,” on the basis that viewing an iPhone was associated with fMRI activity in a brain region previously linked with “love and compassion.” Unfortunately, the same brain area has also been linked with negative emotions like disgust, as well as many other cognitive functions, seriously undermining the claims in the New York Times story. It may turn out to be true that our feelings about iPhones reflect love, or perhaps more likely a kind of dopamine-driven addiction response, but such simplistic analyses as the one the NYT gave such prominence are unlikely to help with understanding that.

However, I think it’s a mistake to confuse the media representation of fMRI research with the field itself. Many researchers design very careful fMRI experiments in which factors of interest are varied while others are controlled, and resulting patterns of brain activity are analysed with statistical caution and interpreted in the light of a broad range of previous findings. This is the kind of work that is moving the field forward, but, like most good science, it is not particularly sexy, and the researchers involved are less keen to make the kinds of extravagant claims that get you into the New York Times. In my view, it’s up to all of us—scientists and journalists—to make sure that the public get to hear more about the good fMRI work, and less about silly iPhone love stories.

2. Is there a specific role for functional neuroimaging in the diagnosis and treatment of addiction?

This isn’t really my area, but I know from talking with colleagues that neuroimaging has certainly contributed to understanding the neurobiology of addiction. For example, a great deal is known about the brain networks and neurotransmitter systems implicated in impulsivity, compulsions, reward processing and other cognitive functions that are relevant to addiction disorders. Such knowledge is obviously very important for informing clinical practice. Whether neuroimaging can also play a role in diagnosis and treatment is, as far as I can tell, less clear. Making accurate diagnoses in an individual on the basis of neuroimaging data requires characterisation of the specific patterns of brain activity exhibited by that individual, which is difficult to achieve. Similar problems afflict attempts to assess the success of different treatments. However, as imaging technology develops and statistical methods are refined, it may be that neuroimaging will be able to offer new insights that contribute to effective diagnosis and treatment.


3. Please explain the basics of state-dependent memory as it relates to alcohol, using your famous example about remembering Brad Pitt’s or Angelina Jolie’s phone number.

Ha! Glad if the example I used has proven memorable. The basic idea of state-dependent memory is that your recollection of a previous event is likely to be better if you’re in the same physiological state as you were when the event occurred. So, to use alcohol as an example, if you’re at a party and happen to drunkenly strike up conversation with Angelina Jolie (or Brad Pitt, if you prefer) and, bowled over by your charm and witty repartee, she tells you her phone number, you may well not remember it when you wake up sober the next morning. However, the evidence from many state-dependent memory studies suggests that you would have a better chance of recalling the number if you got drunk again. The effect doesn’t just apply to alcohol: any physiological state, any emotion we’re feeling, in fact any aspect of the context we’re in when we try to remember, which was also there when we previously experienced an event, will improve our memory for that event. This was described as the ‘encoding specificity principle’ by a great memory researcher, Endel Tulving.

4. Some researchers maintain that mental illness and addiction to drugs and alcohol are not, properly speaking, diseases at all. What’s your stance on the continual battles over the “disease model” of addiction and other disorders like depression?

There are both advantages and disadvantages to the “disease model” of cognitive disorders. For some people, it might be helpful to receive a clinical disease diagnosis and, perhaps, an idea of the therapeutic interventions through which they might go about “recovering” from their condition. Among the disadvantages are that labelling people as “addicts”, for example, can stigmatise those struggling with dependence and might lead to them avoiding responsibility for changing their addictive behaviour because they see diseases as requiring expert intervention. It’s a difficult area, and I can see both sides of the debate.

5. You’ve done neuroscience at Cambridge in the U.K., and at Harvard in the U.S. Is science conducted differently in the two countries?

My experience was that science in the US at that time (in 2000-2001) was different from in the UK. There seemed to be many more opportunities to get involved in interesting projects and to gain access to advanced technical equipment like fMRI scanners than was the case in the UK then. I also noted a difference in the willingness of people who were experts in techniques like fMRI, or in research involving patients with rare brain lesions, to share their expertise and resources in a collaborative way. Fortunately, I’ve found since I’ve been back in the UK a similarly friendly and collaborative environment, particularly in Cambridge now. I think this is partly a result of initiatives to bring researchers together across traditional scientific boundaries, such as Cambridge Neuroscience. However, I think the biggest US/UK difference now is the prevalent feeling that science is valued much more in the US than it is over here. Despite the best efforts of campaigns like Science is Vital, successive UK governments have failed to invest in science to the same degree as other nations, including the US. Particularly in the current financial climate, when significant cuts to science funding have been threatened, this means that morale is low, uncertainty is high, and significant numbers of scientists are deciding to move abroad. I don’t know that I’m ready to join them yet, but if the situation gets much worse, it would have to be something I’d consider.

Photo Credit: http://www.neuroscience.cam.ac.uk
Related Posts Plugin for WordPress, Blogger...